This shows a woman drinking hot drinks with friends

Random acts of kindness cause more buzz than expected

To summarize: People often underestimate the positive emotions and outcomes felt by people who receive random acts of kindness.

resource: University of Texas at Austin

Although they generally increase happiness, acts of kindness like giving a friend a ride or delivering food to a sick family member may be somewhat rare because people underestimate these behaviors, according to a new University of Texas study. The feeling it brings to the recipient. austin.

Research conducted by Amit Kumar, assistant professor of marketing at the UT Austin McCombs School of Business, and Nicholas Epley of the University of Chicago found that while givers tend to focus on what they’re offering or the action they’re performing, takers instead focus on what’s warm. Sensibly, acts of kindness are reminiscent of. This means that the giver’s “misaligned expectations” can become a barrier to performing more prosocial behaviors such as helping, sharing, or donating.

The study is online in advance Journal of Experimental Psychology: General.

To quantify these attitudes and behaviors, the researchers conducted a series of experiments.

For one study, researchers recruited 84 participants in Chicago’s Maggie Daley Park. Participants can choose whether to give a cup of hot chocolate to a stranger from a food kiosk in the park or keep it for themselves. Seventy-five agreed to give up.

The researchers handed hot chocolate to strangers and told them that the study participants chose to serve them the drink. The recipients reported their mood, and the performers expressed what they thought the recipient was feeling after drinking.

Performers underestimate the importance of their performances. They expected recipients’ sentiment to average 2.7, ranging from -5 (more negative than normal) to 5 (more positive than normal), while recipients reported an average of 3.5.

“People didn’t leave the base,” Kumar said. “They know that being nice to people makes them feel good. What we don’t get is how good it really makes people feel.”

The researchers also conducted a similar experiment with cupcakes at the same park. They recruited 200 participants and divided them into two groups. In the control group, 50 participants received a cupcake for participating. They rated their own mood, and 50 others rated how they thought the recipient would feel after receiving the cupcake.

Of the second group of 100 people, 50 were told they could give cupcakes to strangers. They assessed their own mood and the cupcake recipient’s expected mood.

The researchers handed hot chocolate to strangers and told them that the study participants chose to serve them the drink.Image is in the public domain

The researchers found that participants rated the cupcake recipient’s well-being roughly the same whether through a random act of kindness or receiving the cupcake from the researcher. What’s more, recipients who received cupcakes through an act of kindness were happier than recipients in the control group.

“Performers don’t fully consider that their passionate performances provide the value of the performances themselves,” Kumar said. “The fact that you are kind to others adds a lot of value, not just the thing itself.”

In lab experiments, Kumar and Epley added a component to assess the consequences of good intentions. Participants first received a gift from the lab store or a gift from another participant, and then played the game. All participants who received items were told to split $100 between themselves and an unknown study recipient.

The researchers found that recipients of a lab gift through another participant’s random act of kindness were more generous with strangers in the game. They split the $100 more evenly, giving away an average of $48.02 versus an average of $41.20.

“It turns out that generosity can actually be contagious,” Kumar said. “The recipient of prosocial behavior can pay forward. Kindness can actually spread.”

About this Psychology Research News

author: News office
resource: University of Texas at Austin
touch: Press Office – UT Austin
picture: Image is in the public domain

see also

This shows three girls with gorgeous hair

Original research: closed access.
Amit Kumar et al. “A Little Good Will Lead to an Unexpected Long Road: Underestimating the Positive Impact of Kindness on the Receiver”. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General


Abstract

A Little Good Thing Goes a Surprisingly Long Way: Underestimating the Positive Impact of Kindness on Receivers

Random acts of kindness increase the happiness of both givers and receivers, but we found that givers systematically underestimated their positive impact on receivers.

In both field and laboratory settings (Experiments 1a to 2b), those who showed goodwill reported how positively they expected recipients to feel, while recipients reported how they actually felt.

From giving a cup of hot chocolate in the park to giving gifts in the lab, those who did random acts of kindness consistently underestimated the recipient’s positive feelings, arguing that their actions were less valuable than the recipients thought.

The giver’s miscalibrated expectations were partly driven by egocentric bias in the assessment of the behavior itself (Experiment 3). The recipient’s positive response is enhanced by the warmth conveyed by the act of kindness, whereas the giver’s expectations are relatively insensitive to the warmth conveyed by their behavior.

Underestimating the positive effects of random acts of kindness also leads givers to underestimate the behavioral consequences that their prosociality will have on recipients through indirect reciprocity (Experiment 4).

We argue that giver misaligned expectations are important because they impede more frequent engagement in prosocial behaviors in everyday life (Experiments 5a and 5b), which may lead people to miss opportunities to improve their own well-being and that of others—exist

Leave a Comment

Your email address will not be published.